Research paper |
Corresponding author: Sophie M. Dupont ( sophie.dupont93@gmail.com ) Academic editor: Natasha Shelby
© 2019 Sophie M. Dupont, Lauréline Guinnefollau, Christiane Weber, Odile Petit.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
Dupont SM, Guinnefollau L, Weber C, Petit O (2019) Impact of artificial light at night on the foraging behaviour of the European Hamster: consequences for the introduction of this species in suburban areas. Rethinking Ecology 4: 133-148. https://doi.org/10.3897/rethinkingecology.4.36467
|
Among the pressures introduced by urbanization, artificial light at night (ALAN) can be problematic, particularly for nocturnal species. Populations of European hamsters (Cricetus cricetus) have dramatically decreased in France since 1972 because of habitat loss due to urbanisation and changes in agricultural methods. The conservation project Life Alister aims to increase the abundance of this species in suburban areas via hamster release. However, the success of this population-restoration project may be compromised due to the possible effects of ALAN on the daily and seasonal cycles and behaviour of this nocturnal species. To understand how hamsters may respond to relocation, we experimentally studied the impact of ALAN on hamster foraging, a decisive behaviour for survival in natural habitats. This study assessed the behavioural responses of 18 animals when choosing between two food sources of different palatability in two different lighting conditions: artificial light (4 or 40 lux) or darkness. Our results show that hamsters avoided lighting that mimics suburban streetlights, particularly when grooming. Moreover, this study reveals that hamsters do not avoid street-lit areas when highly palatable food is present, suggesting they would be more susceptible to predation under these circumstances. Our results suggest that the adverse effects of ALAN on the behaviour of hamsters released on the outskirts of developed suburban areas could be limited by restricting the number of street lights, moderating the intensity of street lighting, or switching lights off during the hours hamsters are most active. We further recommend that wildlife managers avoid planting plants that are highly palatable to hamsters close to lighting in suburban areas to limit the risk of predation for this species.
Conservation, Cricetus cricetus, Anthropization, street light
Given the increase of the human population living in urban areas (
In this study, we aim to evaluate the possible use of suburban areas as relocation habitats for the protection of European hamsters in France. We focused on areas bordering developed zones (i.e. suburban areas), as these areas have lower levels of perturbation by humans and may thus provide favourable conditions for hamster conservation. However, anthropic disturbance may impact the introduction of hamsters in suburban areas, in particular due to the effects of ALAN. European hamsters are nocturnal and territorial, so ALAN could modify their daily and seasonal cycles and change their behaviour, ultimately impacting individual fitness. For these reasons, we question whether the release of hamsters in suburban areas will actually facilitate restoration of this threatened population. In this paper, to understand how hamsters may respond to relocation, we experimentally studied the impact of artificial lighting on the foraging behaviour of the hamster. With this experiment, we aimed to determine how hamsters bred in captive conditions react when faced with two sources of food (of high or low palatability) under different light intensities typically found in Alsatian cities (in terms of wavelength and light intensities). We hypothesised that the foraging pattern of hamsters will be modified when individuals are confronted with light in the environment and that hamsters will generally show an aversive response to ALAN.
The experimental protocols followed EU Directive 2010/63/EU guidelines for animal experiments and the care and use of laboratory animals, and were approved by the Ethical Committee (CREMEAS) under agreement number APAFIS#3809-2016012713151858.
The 18 hamsters tested in this experiment were adults (2-year old) born and bred in the laboratory breeding centre of the Institut Pluridisciplinaire Hubert Curien in Strasbourg, France. A balanced sex ratio of nine males and nine females was studied. To align with the solitary behaviour of this species, all individuals were housed individually in cages (dimensions 38.5 × 121.5 × 18 cm), which were enriched with nesting materials (cellulose and hay). Animals were maintained at a constant ambient temperature of 18 °C and a relative humidity of 55%. Animals of this centre are bred to be released in the wild for repopulation purposes and are reared under normal photoperiod conditions. Therefore, during the daytime photoperiod, fluorescent lamps emitting 944 lux at mid-height, were synchronised with the sunrise and sunset and with the natural progressive light intensities at this latitude. Kibbles (pellets 105; Safe, Augy, France, composed of 19.3% protein, 54.9% carbohydrates, 5.1% lipids, 4.2% cellulose, 5.0% minerals and 11.5% water) and water were available ad libitum. Hamsters were studied for 8 weeks after emerging from hibernation at the beginning of April 2016. Animals were manipulated using a box to move them from their cages to the experimental test arena in order to reduce stress and limit their contact with humans.
This experiment investigated whether ALAN modifies the foraging patterns of hamsters. To ensure that analogous suburban lighting conditions were successfully reproduced, a lux meter was used prior to testing to measure the light intensity a hamster would experience if 1) directly below the streetlight and 2) in the peripheral zone (at a distance of 15 m) of a streetlight in a suburban area. The distance between two street lamps in a Strasbourg periphery is an average of 35 m. The resulting intensities of 40 lux and 4 lux, respectively, were then reproduced in experimental conditions using light bulbs (Luxeon, type Rebel PC Amber-LXM2-PL010000, dominant wavelength 591.0 nm) that imitate the high-pressure sodium bulbs used in the current lighting system of Strasbourg, Alsace, France.
We built a test arena (dimensions 150 × 80 × 50 cm) composed of two compartments connected via a 10 cm x 10 cm opening, and placed it in a dark room (Figure
A Schematic representation of the test arena used for this experiment. Here, the left compartment is the Illuminated Compartment (IC). The source of light emitted was 4 or 40 lux. The right compartment is the Dark Compartment (DC) and contained an unlit amber LED. The two compartments are connected by an opening in the wall. Hamsters entered the arena through an opening (manually handled) located midway between the compartments. B Schematic representation of our experimental design. We performed 16 trials per individual hamster in a full-factorial design that included laterality (illuminated compartment on Left or Right of the test arena), time of the day (Morning or Afternoon), light intensity (4 lux or 40 lux) and food quality condition (HQF or LQF). We considered only the first trial for each light intensity and for each food quality condition. The four tests we are mentioning at the end of the paragraph were the ones used for the analysis, the others being conducted for checking the potential laterality and time of the day biases (but these trials were not used in the subsequent analyses).
The hamsters were tested in two different conditions (Figure
Each 15-min trial was filmed using an infrared camera placed above the arena. Data were recorded by video monitoring. During the viewing, observers were blinded to the light intensity applied but not to the food quality condition because mealworms and kibbles are easy to distinguish. The time spent by individuals in IC and DC was calculated for each test. The proportion of time of foraging and grooming behaviour was recorded as well as the compartment in which the behaviour was performed was recorded. “Foraging” refers to any behaviour related to the active seeking or manipulation of food. Both mealworms and kibbles require several bites by hamsters in order to be entirely consumed. The term “grooming” refers to cleaning or scratching. Grooming behaviours were recorded because they could reflect individual stress level (
Experiments were conducted between 9:00 am and 5:00 pm. For safety reasons, no testing was permitted by our administration during nights at the lab, so tests were conducted during the day. As this corresponds to the period when the hamsters would usually be asleep, we first ensured that individuals were sufficiently active. For 4 weeks, we filmed hamsters of a breeding room during the whole day and recorded their frequency of activities: mean hourly frequency of exploration was 2.45 ± 0.275; mean hourly frequency of foraging was 1.18 ± 0.202; mean hourly frequency of maintenance was 0.65 ± 0.085; with some individuals being quite active with 15 occurrences of exploration per hour, 12 occurrences of foraging per hour. Moreover, their frenzy observed in the presence of mealworms indicated they were sufficiently active during our trials. In addition, wild hamsters have been reported to display surface activity during the day in urban environments despite that they are nocturnal animals (
All statistical analyses were performed using R.3.1.2 (
The first analysis concerned the time spent in each light condition. We ran two sets of models to explain our variable of interest by separating the two light intensity conditions (4 or 40 lux) in either HQF or LQF conditions. The response variable was expressed as the time spent by individuals in each compartment of the arena divided by the total proportion of time of a trial. We investigated the ability of hamsters to discriminate between our two compartments according to the light intensity thanks to paired Student’s t-tests. We compared the time spent by each individual to chance level (50%: 450 out of 900 recorded seconds) using a binomial test.
The second analysis was the proportion of time spent engaged in behaviours (grooming or foraging). We ran two sets of models to explain our variables of interest in HQF and LQF conditions. The response variables were expressed as: (1) the time spent grooming in the illuminated compartment divided by the total time spent grooming, (2) the time spent foraging in the illuminated compartment of the arena divided by the total time spent foraging. The resulting proportions were considered a binomial distribution. ‘Light intensity’ (4 vs 40 lux) and ‘sex’ (male vs female) were fixed factors. Wald tests were performed to evaluate the effect of each fixed factor on these response variables.
The final analysis sought to determine the effect of food palatability on the time spent foraging in the illuminated compartment of the arena. We again considered the time spent foraging in the illuminated compartment divided by the total time spent foraging. The resulting proportion was considered a binomial distribution. ‘Light intensity’ (4 vs 40 lux), ‘food quality’ (HQF vs LQF) and ‘sex’ (male vs female) were fixed factors. Wald tests were again performed to compare the proportion of time of foraging in each condition and the effect of the light intensity on this variable.
To exclude a possible learning effect, only the first trial of each light intensity per food-quality condition for each hamster was considered in the analysis (N = 18 hamsters for each experimental condition). Analyses thus used data from four trials (LQF and HQF at 4 lux; LQF and HQF at 40 lux). We used Student’s t-test to ensure that the time of day (morning or afternoon) and laterality (illuminated compartment on the left or the right of the arena) had no effect on the proportion of time of foraging and grooming and excluded these variables from our analysis (grooming proportion of time: Student’s t-Test, PTime of day = 0.765; PLaterality = 0.158; Foraging proportion of time: Student’s t-test, PTime of day = 0.114; PLaterality = 0.275). We did not observe an effect of the ‘trial order’, considered as a fixed variable, in any model, so we decided to exclude this variable from our analyses.
In both HQF and LQF conditions, we found that individuals spent significantly more time in the DC than the IC when the IC was illuminated at 40 lux (Table
Mean proportion (%, ± S.D.) of time spent in each compartment (Dark Compartment (DC) or Illuminated Compartment (IC)) of the arena according to the food quality condition (HQF or LQF) and to the light intensity in the IC (4 lux or 40 lux). Proportion of time spent in the DC was significantly different between our two Food quality conditions only when the IC was illuminated at 40 lux.
Proportion of time spent in each compartment (%) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Compartment | Dark compartment | Illuminated compartment | ||
4 lux | 40 lux | 4 lux | 40 lux | |
HQF condition | 50.6 ± 7.5 | 55.9 ± 9.6 | 49.4 ± 7.5 | 44.1 ± 9.6 |
LQF condition | 55.4 ± 13.0 | 59.2 ± 16.2 | 44.6 ± 13.0 | 40.8 ± 16.2 |
The best-fitting model for the proportion of time hamsters spent grooming in the HQF and LQF conditions included only the light intensity (Suppl. material
A In High-quality food condition, proportion of time spent grooming in the Illuminated Compartment (IC) according to the light intensity (4 lux or 40 lux) of this compartment. B In Low-quality food condition, proportion of time spent grooming in the Illuminated Compartment (IC) according to the light intensity (4 lux or 40 lux) of this compartment. The *** indicates the statistical difference at P-value < 0.001.
Mean proportion of time (%, ± S.D.) spent grooming and foraging in both compartments of the arena (DC and IC) according to the Food quality condition (HQF or LQF) and to the light intensity in the Illuminated Compartment (IC, 4 lux or 40 lux). Proportion of grooming and foraging proportion of time in the IC was significantly different between our two experimental light intensities in both Food quality conditions.
Proportion of time spent in each compartment (%) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Behaviour – Testing condition | Dark compartment | Illuminated compartment | ||
4 lux | 40 lux | 4 lux | 40 lux | |
Grooming – HQF condition | 60.1 ± 29.7 | 62.6 ± 37.1 | 39.9 ± 29.7 | 37.3 ± 37.1 |
Grooming – LQF condition | 59.5 ± 29.7 | 66.4 ± 29.8 | 40.5 ± 29.7 | 33.6 ± 29.7 |
Foraging – HQF condition | 43.4 ± 15.6 | 48.3 ± 19.6 | 56.7 ± 15.6 | 51.8 ± 19.6 |
Foraging – LQF condition | 52.9 ± 19.2 | 45.5 ± 22.6 | 47.1 ± 19.2 | 54.5 ± 22.6 |
The best-fitting model for the proportion of time spent foraging in the HQF condition only included the light intensity (Suppl. material
Considering the proportion of time of foraging in the IC, the best-fitting model solely included the food quality. Individuals foraged 12.5% longer in the IC when we placed a mealworm (HQF condition) rather than a kibble (LQF condition) below the light, whatever the light intensity (Wald’s test, z = 6.78, P-value < 0.001; Figure
In this study, we experimentally tried to simulate the impact of public lighting, one of the main pressures encountered by wildlife in suburban habitats, which can entail serious consequences for the food-seeking behaviour of hamsters. Our strategy differs from other conservation studies in Europe (
In this study, we demonstrated that individuals generally avoided the illuminated compartment, particularly when grooming. We also found that avoidance of the illuminated area depended on the light intensity. Indeed, a low intensity of 4 lux (which reproduces the lighting in the peripheral zone of a streetlight) had less of an effect on hamster foraging than a lighting intensity of 40 lux (the equivalent of the lighting directly below a streetlight).
Our results suggest that hamsters living in suburban environments may generally avoid areas with street lighting. Our results confirm our predictions and corroborate the findings of previous studies that demonstrated rodents avoid illuminated areas (
The hamster is a territorial and solitary species, so the probable restriction of resources due to ALAN in suburban areas might lead this animal to increase 1) the time budget allocated to foraging in its home range, and/or 2) the distances it travels – i.e. beyond the borders of its own territory – in order to complete potential food stock, both possibly entailing more frequent intraspecific conflicts. This would increase predation risk and reduce foraging efficiency (i.e. energy intake/energy used for foraging). Consequently, the highly developed lighting system in large suburban areas of Alsace is expected to have a negative impact on hamsters. Limiting the number of lamp posts, applying moderate levels of lighting or switching off streetlights during the hamsters’ period of activity might be a solution to limit the adverse effects of ALAN on the behaviour, body condition and survival of hamsters released on the outskirts of suburban areas. The application of these measures would create favourable conditions for the release of hamsters raised in captivity. More generally, such conditions may be favourable to any wildlife species.
Our study also reveals that although hamsters are able to discriminate between both compartments, they show frenzied and risky behaviours when confronted with highly attractive food. If we extrapolate this lab situation to real suburban conditions, this behaviour could render hamsters more vulnerable to predation. Indeed, instead of hoarding food from the illuminated compartment to the dark compartment as they do classically by going back to their burrow in order to consume the food (
This study is part of a larger research programme that will help providing advice for territorial planning in order to successfully reintroduce this species in suburban areas, and will thus contribute to the restoration of viable populations in France. We believe that testing how captive-bred animals might cope with challenging environments in controlled conditions is a necessary step before releasing animals into the wild for conservation purposes. Another recommended step (which is on the agenda of the French National Plan of Actions,
This work was supported by the LIFE + Biodiversity grant No. LIFE12 BIO/FR/000979 and the Ministère de l’Ecologie, du Développement durable et de l’Energie. We are indebted to Pierre Ulrich, Mark Torzynski and all the members of the Cognitive and Social Ethology group for their assistance in the fieldwork.
Language has been edited by Joanna Lignot, a professional English copy editor.
Conflicts of interest. The funders played no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Supplementary tables
Data type: statistical data
Explanation note: Table S1: Influence of light pollution on the grooming and foraging durations in High-quality food condition. Table S2: Influence of light pollution on the grooming and foraging durations in low-quality food (LQF) condition.